1189:3573
ShareEven Darwin Struggled with Dip and Strike
published Sep 21, 2009I love the image of one of the greatest observational scientists of all time, with the tables in his bedroom all askew, trying to master his brand new clinometer.
Yet he managed to figure it out, and traveled around the globe recording dips and strikes wherever he went. Searching on "dip" in the online database of Darwin Correspondence yields 21 letters referring to geological dip. For example, on 18 April 1835, Darwin wrote from Valparaiso of his observations in the Cordillera:
How did he master this skill on his own in his bedroom, when so many students struggle, even with a professor, TA, and fellow-students to help them? The first quote above holds some clues:
- He actively sought out ongoing mentoring, in this case from a professor from his university days.
- He recognized that he lacked an essential skill, and that he needed practice in a low-stakes situation before going into the field.
- He was able to set up practice exercises for himself, exercises that spanned the full range of possible challenges: "every conceivable angle & direction."
- He was able to monitor his own learning progress, and recognize when he had reached mastery: "I will venture to say I have measured them as accurately as any Geologist going could do."
In other words, he had a strong metacognitive awareness of his own learning process.
Sometimes I think we work too hard at trying to craft ever clearer explanations and ever more engaging learning activities--and not hard enough at figuring out how to help our students teach themselves. I want my students to tip all the tables in their dorm rooms up at weird angles, metaphorically speaking. Even trickier, I want them to come up with the idea themselves, to tip up the tables at weird angles.
Sources:
- Darwin's clinometer and the first quote above are part of the Darwin exhibit, assembled by the American Museum of Natural History, in collaboration with the Museum of Science, Boston; The Field Museum, Chicago; the Royal Ontario Museum, Toronto, Canada; and the Natural History Museum, London, England. I saw them when the exhibit was in New York in 2006. Your next chance to see them in person will be at the San Diego Natural History Museum, November 7, 2009 through February 28, 2010. It is worth going!
- Beautiful (but copyrighted) photograph of the actual clinometer used by Darwin on his voyage aboard the Beagle.
- Online interactive from the Sedgwick Museum on Dawin's Tools of the Trade
- Hemler, D., & Repine, T. (2006). Teachers doing science: An authentic geology research experience for teachers. Journal of Geoscience Education, 54, 93-102.
- Lynn S. Liben, Kim A. Kastens, and Adam E. Christensen. Water-level Task Performance and Gender are Linked to Success in Geology (2.2MB). Poster presented at the 18th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, New York, May 26, 2006.
Even Darwin Struggled with Dip and Strike --Discussion
A reader called my attention to the blog maintained by the curator of the Darwin exhibit at the American Museum of Natural History, Niles Eldredge, who wrote about Darwin's preparations for his voyage on the Beagle at:
www.nileseldredge.com/darwin_blogs_025.htm
Two interesting points:
* Darwin spent a week in the field with geologist Adam Sedgwick (Professor of Geology at Cambridge) training in field geology before the self-teaching episode with clinometer in the bedroom.
* "To the end of his life, Darwin regarded himself first and foremost as a geologist."
1189:3574
Share1189:3583
ShareI downloaded your poster on strike and dip/water level task performance. Very interesting! Do you know if there's any research on whether interventions can help students who do poorly on the WLT to improve their abilities to visualize the horizontal?
Cognitive aspects are clearly at issue here, but I'd like to add that *language* is an unexpectedly large barrier, as well, to teaching three dimensional thinking in structural geology.
I had taught structural geology at Hamilton for quite a number of years before I tumbled to the fact that a surprising number of my students do not have accurate internalized definitions for the words horizontal, vertical, perpendicular, and parallel. Early in the semester, I always ask students on a homework assignment to write definitions of the four words, and, in a class of 20, only a couple will be really clear and spot on. Most are hazy about the distinctions, and a good 1/3 are disastrously wrong (from the point of view of learning structural geology). The most common misconception is that something that is horizontal is oriented east-west and that something vertical is oriented north-south. The same problem occurs with perpendicular and parallel, which many of my students think of as being absolute orientations (i.e., they'll say something like, "The line is perpendicular." - not perpendicular *to* something else, just "perpendicular").
So, it's difficult to teach strike and dip successfully if you don't realize that a significant portion of the class visualizes east-west when you say horizontal. Or that they visualize a vertical line when you say that the dip line is perpendicular to the strike line....
These are very firmly held mental definitions. I can ask students mid to late semester in a structural geology course, and some students will still spontaneously give the "horizontal = EW" definition. And, the more you ask them to talk in class or lab, the more likely you are to hear them say things that make it clear that their ideas of horizontal, vertical, perpendicular, and parallel are not the same as yours.
And we typically use the terms "inclined" and "inclination" when we're talking about dip, and, to a structural geologist, inclination is always measured in the down-dip direction. But most students visualize an incline as going *up*, not down, so when we talk about the angle of inclination, they're visualizing up not down.
On another thing - the tendency for students to put either the strike or dip line parallel to the edge of something (outcrop, clipboard, etc.) is a strong one. A couple of years ago, I got a bunch of cardboard circles for them to practice strike and dip on so that they wouldn't be distracted by the edges.
1189:3637
ShareI have to admit that I played with my Freiberger compass (dip, dip direction) a bit before I went into the field when I switched to it from a Brunton, yet I don't feel that I have conceptual issues understanding strike and dip.
1189:3638
SharePerhaps "struggled with" was an overly dramatic choice of words on my part. Certainly compared with students who can't figure out how to place the compass on the dipping surface (as you described), or who aren't clear on the meaning of horizontal (as Barb Tewksbury described), Darwin was on top of the concept and the skill. What Darwin's letter conveyed to me was that he found the process of measuring dip and strike to be far from non-trivial, something that needed to be consciously and purposefully practiced again and again with different surfaces inclined different ways.
-----
Hi Barb,
Thanks for your wide-ranging and interesting questions and observations.
With respect to whether there are interventions that can improve the ability of students who do poorly on the WLT task to visualize the horizontal, performance on the WLT itself is very resistant to improvement through instruction. As far as I know, no one has tested whether improvements in WLT performance translate into better understanding of horizontal in a real world 3-D context.
Lynn Liben and colleagues tried many interventions for the classical WLT (e.g. stating that the water will remain horizontal, having students manipulate real bottles of water), with only limited improvement. They eventually found a form of intervention that worked pretty well (rule was stated twice, then presented as a fill-in question, then answer to fill-in question was given, then asked again as an open ended question at the end). Liben, L. S., and Golbeck, S. L., 1984, Performance on Piagetian horizontality and verticality tasks: Sex-related differences in knowledge of relevant physical phenomena: Develpmental Psychology, v. 20, p. 595-606.
With respect to the idea that *language* is a problem, there is a really interesting but somewhat dense paper by Talmy, L., 1983, How language structures space, in H. L. Pick, J., and Acredolo, L. P., eds., Spatial orientation: Theory, research and application: New York, Plenum, p. 225-282. Basically, he says that the language we use informs and shapes how we think about space. Which means that you are right to be seriously concerned about your students' ability to understand spatially-demanding concepts if they are using and understanding spatial language differently than you do.
So after you give your students the homework question to define horizontal, vertical, perpendicular and parallel, and find that 1/3 of them are seriously wrong, what do you do next to try and get them all using the same language? Also, have you ever asked them to define "north," "south," "east," or "west"? If they define "horizontal" as east-west, I wonder if they also define east and west in some strange way?
1189:3647
ShareI do ask them to define east, west, north, and south, and the answers are commonly right, left, up, and down, or something like "north is perpendicular to the horizontal". So, language is a real issue.
My strategy is to talk about it in class and reinforce the correct language by specifically saying what is and isn't correct terminology when I use it; I also give them straw sentences with terminology, and we work through whether the terminology is correct or not; and I correct students in class (gently) when they use the wrong terms. The main thing is recognizing that many students aren't using the terms the same way we do. If an instructor is just lecturing to students, instead of having conversations, that might never emerge.
1189:3676
Share