An Interactive Introduction to Randomized Control Trials

This page is authored by Utteeyo Dasgupta, Franklin and Marshall College. It is based on an original activity by Subha Mani, Fordham University.
Author Profile
This material is replicated on a number of sites as part of the SERC Pedagogic Service Project
Initial Publication Date: April 29, 2013

Summary

The economics discipline is now flush with impact evaluation exercises especially in the area of Development economics. The primary tool of impact evaluation is using the RCT framework. However, the RCT procedure, and its effects are not very intuitive when introduced through standard chalk and talk methods. The Classroom RCT Game introduced here provides an interactive introduction to the technique of randomized evaluation, and demonstrates the Average Treatment Effect using student generated data from the activity. The experiment is straightforward to implement, and provides students experiential learning opportunities to the nuts and bolts of impact evaluation.

Share your modifications and improvements to this activity through the Community Contribution Tool »

Learning Goals

By participating in the classroom game students get a first hand insight into a randomized program evaluation that helps them to understand the effect of a treatment, and the critical notion of Average Treatment Effect (ATE). Results from the exercise can further help in understanding the fact that ATE is a measure of the overall impact of an intervention, i.e., the outcome in terms of a distribution, and does not necessarily lead to all participants experiencing the improvement found in the average treatment effect. Students can appreciate not only the nuances of RCT literature better, but they also get a first-hand exposure to a simple experiment design.

Context for Use

Impact evaluation has become a common tool in economics. Specially, Randomized Control Trials (RCT) are now the main investigative tool for development economists. For example, randomized control trials were implemented to understand the effectiveness of interventions such as the conditional cash transfer programs introduced to improve children's test scores, completed grades and enrollment in Mexico, deworming pills programs on children's health and schooling, and effectiveness of microcredit programs in alleviating poverty.

The exercise suggested here can provide an interactive introduction to the topic in courses such as Development Microeconomics, Public Policy and Economics of Development, Economic Development and Growth, Econometrics and Impact Evaluation, and Community Economic Development. By participating in the experiment the students have the opportunity not only to learn the core measure in any evaluation program (the Average Treatment Effect), they also get exposed to an intuitive understanding of the impact evaluation technique. The Classroom RCT Game does not need to be restricted just to undergraduate introductions to the topic of randomized evaluation. The game can be used in a graduate course as well, since it can be time-consuming if not impossible to take the whole class to the field to provide a first-hand exposure to an actual randomized evaluation program. Additionally, the idea of control and trial is now common enough in courses other than development economics. Courses in behavioral economics and experimental economics routinely have a topic on measurement and experiment design. Here again, the classroom game can provide a personal experience into the design and process of an experiment which can make the logic of designing an experiment – to evaluate and estimate "treatment differentials" more vivid to the participating student.


The experiment takes about 35-45 minutes for a class of 20 students. The activity is introduced before starting with lectures on RCT. Some simple preparation before hand is needed as indicated below.

Description and Teaching Materials

Preparation

1. A set of poker chips of two different colors. Alternatively one can use two different suits from a deck of cards (Diamonds and Clubs).

2. A list of words with associated meaning written next to each. A GRE wordlist would be perfect. We use terms in economics as an example Wordlist (Acrobat (PDF) 55kB Feb18 13) here. Multiple copies of the list are needed to distribute to about half the students in class.
3. A quiz comprising of these words is needed. See example Quiz (Acrobat (PDF) 67kB Feb18 13). Copies of the quiz need to be distributed to all students.

Overview of the experiment

In the experiment students participate in an intervention where a random subset of them are exposed to a list of words with associated meanings, while the rest are not. All students then participate in a quiz on word meanings containing these words. It is expected that due to the "intervention", the students exposed to the wordlist will have a higher average score than the students who were not exposed to the wordlist. The random placement of students in a treatment and the control group ensures that pre-existing differences are averaged out between the two groups.

The objective of this activity is to provide students an intuitive understanding of how treatment differences arise, and the concept of Average Treatment Effect (ATE). The Average Treatment Effect is the foremost variable of interest in any randomized control trial, since it captures the impact of the treatment on the outcome-variable of interest.

Description of the classroom activity

1. Students need to be placed randomly in a Treatment and a Control group, first. To construct the treatment and the control group, poker chips are handed out to the students at the beginning of the experiment. Students with red chips are assigned to the treatment group and are asked to sit on the right side of the classroom. Students with white chips are assigned to the control group and are asked to sit on the left side of the classroom. Handing out the chips provides a useful depiction of random assignment into groups, a critical methodology for disentangling treatment effects from pre-existing differences.

2. Each student in the Treatment group is given a copy of "Wordlist" to review for five minutes. Students in the Control group do not have any task at that time. Alternatively, one can distribute pieces of papers to all students where students in the Treatment group receive the wordlist while students in the control group receive blank sheets.

3. At the end of the review period, the instructor collects back the wordlists from the treatment group, distributes the "Quiz" to all students in the treatment as well as the control group. They are allowed five minutes to complete the quiz.

4. At the end of five minutes, the instructor reads out the correct answers for students to score their tests. The students are asked to write their total points on the left hand corner of the test – a point for each correct answer.

5. The instructor collects the scored quiz sheets and computes the average score for the treatment group, and the average score for the control group.

6. The difference in the average quiz scores of the two groups is the Average Treatment Effect of the intervention. A simple excel graph can be used for visual elaboration. This can be readily done using an excel sheet. Further, at this point it can be very useful to show the overall distribution of scores for the treatment and the control groups. Overlaying the two distributions can provide an excellent demonstration of the fact that although there can be some overlap of the scores for the two groups, there is a shift of the distribution for the treatment group to the right. This visual description can bring home a particularly interesting point that not everyone in the treatment group improve compared to the control group.




Teaching Notes and Tips

Using the results

The natural way one can use the activity is to start with the Excel Graphs (Excel 2007 (.xlsx) 33kB Feb18 13) of the computed results before introducing ATE formally (see concept below).
The fact that the students themselves have generated the data allows them to identify with all the components of the experiment design readily, and allows the instructor to describe and define the core measure and other subsequent measures more naturally (see Mani and Dasgupta 2010 for some extensions).

Note, our intervention can also be used to allow students revise concepts that have been just covered in the lectures.

Post activity discussion

1. Definition of ATE: Consider a pool of applicants (N) for a job training program. A randomly selected subset NT gets assigned to the treatment group (T), and receives the treatment (for example: the job training program). The remaining sample NC = N-NT gets assigned to the control (C) group which does not receive the training. In our example we are interested in measuring the impact of the training program on some measurable outcome variable (Y) such as wage earnings. The Average Treatment Effect (ATE) measures the overall impact of a program on an observable outcome variable. Under perfect compliance, it is defined to be the difference in the empirical means of the outcome variable (Y collected at the end of the program) between the treatment and the control group. Thus, under perfect compliance,

ATE = Y-BART- Y-BARC,

where Y-BART is the sample mean of the outcome variable for everyone in the treatment group and Y-BARC is the sample mean of the outcome variable for everyone in the control group.

2. Randomization: It is also educative to bring up the issue of randomization at the end of the activity. In particular, it is useful to point out that the impact evaluation technique depends on how one addresses the issue of missing data to evaluate the impact. The problem arises because one cannot observe the outcomes of program participation unless there are beneficiaries. The beneficiaries however, need to be compared with the right counterfactuals, that is subjects who could be beneficiaries, but are not. Without information on counterfactual, the next best thing is to compare outcomes of treated individuals or households with those of a comparison group that has not been treated. In doing so, one attempts to pick a comparison group that is very similar to the treated groups such that those who received treatment would have had outcomes similar to those in the comparions group in absence of treatment. Randomization is one of the methods used to address the issue of the missing counterfactuals provided the program impacts are examined at the level of randomization. Here, careful selection of control areas is also important in ensuring comparability with participant areas and ultimately calculating the treatment effect. For example, in the classroom exercise described above, instead of distributing the chips to the class once they are seated, if chips were handed out as students walked into the class room, there could be a potential loss of control in randomly assigning chips to students to place them into treatment and control groups; one can potentially think that late arrivers to class might not be the ideal counterfactual group for the early arrivers in case they end up being over represented in either the treatment or the control group due to the assignment method. An obvious way to overcome this would then be to hand out the red and white chips alternatively.

3. Other considerations: The instructor should possibly touch upon issues of IRB protocols, issues of fairness, and criteria for good RCTs in subsequent lecture discussions. See Khandker, Koolwal, Samad(2010) for useful discussions on these issues.

Related Reading

After introducing the formal concept of ATE, the instructor can follow up on some of the classic studies below to discuss some of the actual applications of RCT and the usage of average treatment effects.

Conditional cash transfer program: In an effort to improve children's schooling outcomes (test scores, completed grades, and enrollment), cash transfer payments have been provided as incentives to parents' who send their children regularly to school. A randomized control trial implemented to understand the effectiveness of conditional cash transfers find positive association between the program and - schooling enrollment, and completed grades of schooling in Mexico (Parker, Rubalcava, & Teruel, 2008; Behrman, Sengupta & Todd, 2005).

Deworming pills program: In an attempt to improve children's health and schooling, Miguel & Kremer (2004) and Bobonis, Miguel, & Puri-Sharma (2006) evaluate the effectiveness of providing deworming pills to school age children using a randomized control trial. Both papers find positive impact of the intervention on children's schooling attendance.

Microfinance program: Banerjee et. al (2009) conduct the first randomized evaluation study to assess the effectiveness of microcredit on poverty. The authors find that increased access to microcredit is associated with increased expenditure on durable goods though, not associated with improvements in average household per capita expenditure – an important measure of well-being.


Assessment

One interesting way the students can be assessed at the end of the topic is to ask them to submit a research proposal of their own using the RCT framework. The Proposal (Acrobat (PDF) 55kB May9 13) gives an outline for such an exercise.

References and Resources

This activity was based on the paper Mani, S., and Dasgupta, U. (2010): "Explaining Randomized Evaluation Techniques Using Classroom Games. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1676876 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1676876

References

  1. Banerjee, A.V., Duflo, E., Glennerster, R., & Kothari, D. (2010). Improving immunisation coverage in rural India: clustered randomised controlled evaluation of immunisation campaigns with and without incentives. BMJ2010; 340:c2220.
  2. Behrman, J. R., Sengupta, P., & Todd, P. (2005). Progressing through PROGRESA: An Impact Assessment of a School Subsidy Experiment in Rural Mexico, Economic Development and Cultural Change, University of Chicago Press, vol. 54(1), pages 237-75, October.
  3. Bobonis, G. J., Miguel, E., & Puri-Sharma, C. (2006). Anemia and School Participation. J. Human Resources, XLI(4), 692–721.
  4. Miguel, E., & Kremer, M. (2004). Worms: Identifying Impacts on Education and Health in the Presence of Treatment Externalities. Econometrica, 72(1), 159–217.
  5. Parker, S. W., Rubalcava, L., & Teruel, G. (2008). Evaluating Conditional Schooling and Health Programs. Handbook of Development Economics.
  6. "Khandker, Shahidur R.; Koolwal, Gayatri B.; Samad, Hussain A.. 2010. Handbook on Impact Evaluation : Quantitative Methods and Practices. © World Bank. https://openknowledge.worldbank.org/handle/10986/2693.